BelievePerform - The UK's leading Sports Psychology Website
  • Resources
  • Blog
    • Performance
    • Coaching
    • Well Being
    • Education
    • News
  • Contact Us
  • Membership
  • Login
Memberships Login

Articles / Attentional focus in endurance running.

  • Performance
  • Coaching
  • Well Being
  • Education
  • News
  • Performance
  • Coaching
  • Well Being
  • Education
  • News

Attentional focus in endurance running.

Written by Charles Steward

Defining attentional focus.

Attentional focus in athletes can be split into two broad strategies, that of association and dissociation (Lind et al. 2009). A range of terms for association and dissociation are in the literature, these include internal focus, attention, and redefinition for association and external focus, distraction and avoidance for dissociation, although the constructs themselves are extremely similar (Brewer & Buman 2006). Attentional focus is a critical psychological element within a wide range of endurance sports such as running (Summers et al. 1982; Schomer 1986; Goode & Roth 1993; Masters & Ogles 1998; Stevinson & Biddle 1998; LaCaille et al. 2004; Buman et al. 2008; Tenenbaum & Connolly 2008), rowing (Connolly & Janelle 2003; Longman et al. 2014), and cycling (Stanley et al. 2007). Originally Schomer (1986) developed attentional strategies through the single dimension of task relatedness, merging association and dissociation with attentional width for example, whether attentional focus is broad or narrow and the direction of attention for example, towards internal or external signals. Associative thoughts consequently contained thoughts that were task related such as bodily sensations, conditions and pacing with dissociative thoughts containing thoughts that were non task related such as daydreaming, problem solving and admiring the view. However, it had been established that 63% of thoughts existing in participants were not classified as the thoughts fell in between associative and dissociative, for instance a thought that was associative as it was related to the task, but also dissociative as it acted as distraction from the distress during the run (Summers et al. 1982). This led to attentional focus being re-developed by Stevinson & Biddle (1998) who proposed a two-dimensional classification system as seen below.

External Task-relevant  

Outward monitoring: For example, strategy, mile markers, water stations,  split times, route and conditions.

External Task-irrelevant

Outward distraction: For example, scenery, spectators, other runnersand the environment.

Internal Task-relevant  

Inward monitoring: For example, breathing, muscle soreness, thirst, fatigue, perspiration, blisters and nausea.

Internal Task-irrelevant

Inward distraction: For example, daydreams, imagining music, maths puzzles, philosophy and religion.

A range of methods to measure attentional focus in runners have been utilised such as recording devices (Schomer et al. 1986), structured verbal interviews (McDonald & Kirkby 1995), online interviews (Buman et al. 2008), and questionnaires (Goode & Roth 1993; Stevinson & Biddle 1998). However, it must be acknowledged that each has methodological limitations. The utilisation of recording devices to record spoken thoughts whilst the runners ran could result in participants discomfort and potentially cause a decrease in performance level, subsequently experimentally-imposed dissociative thoughts may be provoked (Masters & Ogles 1998). Self-report methods can cause participants to be selective in reporting because of potential embarrassment. Furthermore there is the possibility of vague thoughts being incorrectly classified by researchers (Longman et al. 2014). Additionally the use of recalled attention focus has the potential to be inaccurate due to the distorting effect of time on memory (Schomer et al. 1986).

Attentional focus in competitive endurance performance.

The research has indicated that elite endurance runners and experienced runners prefer to utilise associative strategies during performance (Masters & Lambert 1989; Silva & Appelbaum 1989; McDonald & Kirkby 1995), this is most likely due to the elite performers often being more experienced, and thus are able to afford an associate strategy because of the physical superiority. Furthermore an increase in exercise intensity has demonstrated this transition to associative thoughts (Hutchinson & Tenenbaum 2007; Tenenbaum & Connolly 2008; Razon et al. 2009). The significance of employing associative strategies to increase performance level during phases of high intensity is widely recognised (Birrer & Morgan 2010). Via associative strategies, performers can monitor sensory feedback to improve performance by modification of racing strategy or pacing (LaCaille et al. 2004). For instance, when increasing speed to keep up with the leading pack in an endurance race, if the runner does not ‘check up’ on forms of physical pain, an incorrect pacing decision may be made. This may result in the performer decreasing pacing drastically due to fatigue at a later stage in the race or completing the race sub maximally. These associative strategies have been deemed a predictor of success in endurance performance (Silva & Appelbaum 1989). Alternatively in weaker runners dissociation appears more frequently, being more effective than association, these results are likely influenced by the weaker performers often being less experienced (Brewer & Buman 2008). The use of dissociation enables performers to desensitise oneself from sensory feedback, thus decreasing the level of pain, discomfort and monotony during performance (Schomer 1986). Furthermore dissociative strategies have also been seen to increase when the duration of the exercise increases; which often corresponds with a decrease in exercise intensity at maximal exertion, the desensitising results in a reduction in the perception of exertion (Stanley et al. 2007; Lohse & Sherwood 2011). This has been shown to be crucial in performers decision making processes, which can be the difference between success and failure through adapting pacing strategies and even the choice whether to cease or prolong performance (Morgan 1981). For instance, when taking part in the final stages of an endurance race when physical discomfort is high, if the performer does not ‘distract’ ones-self this may cause a sub optimal pacing strategy or even cause the performer to give up and quit the race due to the perceived pain.

Coping strategies for endurance performance.

The inclusion of psychological skills training, for example, self-talk and imagery (Masters & Lambert 1989), has been recommended alongside attentional focus manipulation, in particular for non-elite endurance performers to prevent the interpretation of exertion and pain in a more emotional manner (Masters & Lambert 1989). A study by Blanchfield et al. (2013) recognised that in particular motivational self-talk in endurance performance can cause an enhanced perceived ability of the performer to sustain the necessary power output for an extended period of time. Thus the cognitive effect of motivational self-talk led to a reduction in rating of perceived exertion therefore, the point at which maximal effort was believed to have took place was delayed. Examples of motivational self-talk used in this study including; “drive forward”, “you’re doing well”. In terms of attentional focus manipulation an early study by Stevinson & Biddle (1998) consisting of 66 non-elite London marathon runners determined that inward monitoring is crucial for sustaining physical awareness, changes in pacing and monitoring fluid intake thus increasing comfort and performance. This being said, over attention will amplify discomfort levels resulting in ‘hitting the wall’ at an earlier stage of running. Thus regular but short in duration ‘check ups’ instead on continuous monitoring is recommended.

References

Show all
Birrer, D., & Morgan, G. (2010) Psychological skills training as a way to enhance an athlete's performance in high‐intensity sports. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports. [Online] 20(2), 78-87. Available from: SPORTDiscus [Accessed 28th September 2014].

Blanchfield, A. W., Hardy, J., de Morree, H. M., Staiano, W., & Marcora, S. M. (2013) Talking yourself out of exhaustion: the effects of self-talk on endurance performance. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. [Online] 46(5), 998-1007. Available from: ResearchGate [Accessed 30th September 2014].

Brewer, B. W., & Buman, M. P. (2006) Attentional focus and endurance performance: review and theoretical integration. Kinesiologia Slovenica. [Online] 12(2), 82-97. Available from: http://www.kinsi.si/upload/clanki/72750_144.pdf [Accessed 30th September 2014].

Buman, M. P., Omli, J. W., Giacobbi Jr, P. R., & Brewer, B. W. (2008) Experiences and coping responses of “hitting the wall” for recreational marathon runners. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. [Online] 20(3), 282-300. Available from: SPORTDiscus [Accessed 29th September 2014].

Connolly, C., & Janelle, C. (2003) Attentional strategies in rowing: Performance, perceived exertion, and gender considerations. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. [Online] 15(3), 195-212. Available from: SPORTDiscus [Accessed 26th September 2014].

Goode, K. T., & Roth, D. L. (1993) Factor analysis of cognitions during running: Association with mood change. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. [Online] 15, 375-375. Available from: SPORTDiscus [Accessed 29th September 2014].

Hutchinson, J. C., & Tenenbaum, G. (2007) Attention focus during physical effort: The mediating role of task intensity. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. [Online] 8(2), 233-245. Available from: Science Direct [Accessed 30th September 2014].

LaCaille, R. A., Masters, K. S., & Heath, E. M. (2004) Effects of cognitive strategy and exercise setting on running performance, perceived exertion, affect, and satisfaction. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. [Online] 5(4), 461-476. Available from: Science Direct [Accessed 25th September 2014].

Lind, E., Welch, A. S., & Ekkekakis, P. (2009) Do ‘Mind over muscle’ strategies work? Sports Medicine. [Online] 39(9), 743-764. Available from: SPORTDiscus [Accessed 26th September 2014].

Lohse, K. R., & Sherwood, D. E. (2011) Defining the focus of attention: effects of attention on perceived exertion and fatigue. Frontiers in Psychology. [Online] 2, 332. Available from: PubMed [Accessed 30th September 2014].

Longman, D., Hutchinson, J. C., Stock, J. T., & Wells, J. C. (2014) Attentional strategies during rowing. Journal of Basic and Applied Sciences. [Online] 10, 321-33. Available from: http://www.lifescienceglobal.com/pms/index.php/jbas/article/view/2183/1293 [Accessed 27th September 2014].

Masters, K. S., & Lambert, M. J. (1989) The relations between cognitive coping strategies, reasons for running, injury, and performance of marathon runners. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. [Online] 11(2), 161-170. Available from: ResearchGate [Accessed 28th September 2014].

Masters, K. S., & Ogles, B. M. (1998) Associative and dissociative cognitive strategies in exercise and running: 20 years later, what do we know? Sport Psychologist. [Online] 12, 253-270. Available from: ResearchGate [Accessed 30th September 2014].

Masters, K. S., & Ogles, B. M. (1998) The relations of cognitive strategies with injury, motivation, and performance among marathon runners: Results from two studies. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. [Online] 10(2), 281-296. Available from: Taylor & Francis [Accessed 28th September 2014].

McDonald, D., & Kirkby, R. J. (1995) Use of dissociation strategies when running becomes difficult: Levels of ability and gender differences. European Journal of High Ability. [Online] 6(1), 73-81. Available from: Taylor & Francis [Accessed 28th September 2014].

Morgan, W. P. (1981) The 1980 CH McCloy Research Lecture: Psychophysiology of self-awareness during vigorous physical activity. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. [Online] 52(3), 385-427. Available from: Taylor & Francis [Accessed 28th September 2014].

Razon, S., Basevitch, I., Land, W., Thompson, B., & Tenenbaum, G. (2009) Perception of exertion and attention allocation as a function of visual and auditory conditions. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. [Online] 10(6), 636-643. Available from: Science Direct [Accessed 30th September 2014].

Schomer, H. (1986) Mental strategies and the perception of effort of marathon runners. International Journal of Sport Psychology. [Online] 17 (1), 41-59. Available from: http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1987-14208-001 [Accessed 30th September 2014].

Silva III, J. M., & Appelbaum, M. I. (1989) Association-dissociation patterns of United States Olympic marathon trial contestants. Cognitive Therapy and Research. [Online] 13(2), 185-192. Available from: ResearchGate [Accessed 28th September 2014].

Stanley, C. T., Pargman, D., & Tenenbaum, G. (2007) The effect of attentional coping strategies on perceived exertion in a cycling task. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. [Online] 19(3), 352-363. Available from: SPORTDiscus [Accessed 28th September 2014].

Stevinson, C. D., & Biddle, S. J. (1998) Cognitive orientations in marathon running and" hitting the wall". British Journal of Sports Medicine. [Online] 32(3), 229-234. Available from: SPORTDiscus [Accessed 25th September 2014].

Summers, J. J., Sargent, G. I., Levey, A. J., & Murray, K. D. (1982) Middle-aged, non-elite marathon runners: A profile. Perceptual and Motor Skills. [Online] 54(3), 963-969. Available from: http://www.amsciepub.com/doi/abs/10.2466/pms.1982.54.3.963 [Accessed 30th September 2014].

Tenenbaum, G., & Connolly, C. T. (2008) Attention allocation under varied workload and effort perception in rowers. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. [Online] 9(5), 704-717. Available from: Science Direct [Accessed 28th September 2014].
Author

Charles Steward

22 years old. Sport and Exercise Science. University of Worcester. Key interest in the self-regulation of performance.

View profile

Sign up for  membership…

and gain unlimited access to all courses, training plans, and supporting materials (including our 1000+ infographics!)

£99/year
for individuals

£1200/year
for schools and organisations

Get started

Popular Articles

Identify your fear of re-injury and overcome it!

Optimising performance and injury prevention by sleep monitoring in adolescent athletes

What does the future hold for Rory McIlroy: Hero or Zero?

View all articles

OUR PARTNERS

BelievePerform - The UK's leading Sports Psychology Website

© BelievePerform 2025

Follow Us

Online Courses

  • Building psychological skills into your training plan
  • Keeping your confidence during challenging times
  • Keeping essential mental health habits going during isolation

Top Articles

  • You’re not you when you’re worried
  • Mental Health Is A Journey
  • SuperSport School Partnership
  • Meet the Team
  • Contributors
  • Privacy Policy
  • Terms & Conditions
  • Author Login
Access to 1000+ infographics Become a Member